Links between motivation and metacognition and achievement in cognitive performance among primary school pupils

Authors

DOI: https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.383941
Keywords: Cognitive performance, Motivation, Metacognition, Goal motivation, Academic achievement, Primary school

Abstract

Sundry studies have emphasised the importance of motivation in cognitive performance, as well as its link to metacognitive development. The objective here is to explore the motivation variables that influence the performance of cognitive tasks, along with their effect on metacognition. The study sample consists of 354 primary school pupils aged 8-11 at a state school in the Community of Madrid (Spain). The following measuring in-struments have been used for studying the variables: Motivación para el Aprendizaje y la Ejecución (MAPE-I) [Motivation for Learning and Execu-tion], a questionnaire on motivation variables; the Junior Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (Jr. MAI), for metacognitive variables, and the bat-tery of tests in the Evaluación Neuropsicológica de las Funciones Ejecutivas en Ni-ños (ENFEN) [Neuropsychological Assessment of Executive Functions in Children], for assessing cognitive performance. The results reveal a signifi-cant effect between the motivation for focusing on learning and cognitive performance in the tasks that involve inhibition, flexibility, and working memory. We also find a significant effect between increased effort and knowledge regulation. Furthermore, we analyse the relations between the variables by school year to determine the age effect. We evaluate the re-sults’ academic implications.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Author Biography

Natalia Lara Nieto-Márquez, Universidad Camilo José Cela

Natalia Lara currently works at Smile and Learn Digital Creations with the project IND2017/SOC-7874. Her studies are linked with Camilo José Cela University with a 'Technology education PhD'. Her research interests are self regulated learning (SRL), game based learning (GBL), metacognition, motivation and executive functions. She develops digital material for learning Science at Smile and Learn Digital Creations.

https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Natalia_Lara5

https://www.linkedin.com/in/n-lara/?locale=en_US

References

Ames, C., y Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: students’ learning strategies and motivation processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80 (3), 260-267. Recuperado de: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.260.

Bahri, A., y Corebima, A. D. (2015). The contribution of learning motivation and metacognitive skill on cognitive learning outcome of students within different learning strategies. Journal of Baltic Science Education, 14(4), 487-500. Recuperado de: http://eprints.unm.ac.id/11457/1/Bahri_JBSE_Vol.14_No.4%20487-500..pdf

Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Educational psychologist, 28 (2), 117-148. doi: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3

Barca-Lozano, A., Almeida, L. S., Porto-Rioboo, A. M., Peralbo, M., y Brenlla-Blanco, J. C. (2012). Motivación escolar y rendimiento: impacto de metas académicas, de estrategias de aprendizaje y autoeficacia. Anales de psicología, 28(3), 848-859. doi: https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.28.3.156101

Bembenutty, H. (2007). Self-regulation of learning and academic delay of gratification: Gender and ethnic differences among college students. Journal of advanced academics, 18(4), 586-616. doi: https://doi.org/10.4219/jaa-2007-553

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., y Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327–336. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007.

Borkowski, J. G., Chan, L. K., y Muthukrishna, N. (2000). A process-oriented model of metacognition: Links between motivation and executive functioning. Issues in the Measurement of Metacognition. 2. Recuperado de: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/burosmetacognition/2

Brocki, K. C., y Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental neuropsychology, 26(2), 571-593. doi: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2602_3

Coutinho, S. y Neuman, G. (2008). A model of metacognition, achievement goal orientation, learning style and self-efficacy. Learning environments research, 11 (2), 131-151. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10984-008-9042-7

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135-168. doi: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American psychologist, 41(10). 1040-1048. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1040

Efklides, A. (2011). Interactions of metacognition with motivation and affect in self-regulated learning: The MASRL model. Educational psychologist, 46 (.1), 6-25. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2011.538645

Elliott, E. S., y Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(1), 5. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.1.5

Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American psychologist, 34(10), 906-901. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906

Follmer, D. J., y Sperling, R. A. (2016). The mediating role of metacognition in the relationship between executive function and self-regulated learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(4), 559–575. doi:10.1111/bjep.12123

Gaeta, M. L. (2006). Estrategias de autorregulación del aprendizaje: contribución de la orientación de meta y la estructura de metas del aula. Revista Electrónica Interuniversitaria de Formación del Profesorado, 9(1). Recuperado de: http://www.aufop.com/aufop/home/ - Consulted march 20, 2010

Gaeta, M. L., Teruel, M. P., y Orejudo, S. (2012). Motivational, volitional and metacognitive aspects of self regulated learning. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 10(1), 73-94. Recuperado de: http://investigacion-psicopedagogica.com/revista/articulos/26/english/Art_26_640.pdf

García, T., Rodríguez, C., González-Castro, P., y Álvarez-García, D. (2016). Metacognition and executive functioning in Elementary School. Anales de psicología, 32(2), 474-483. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.32.2.202891

Garcia, T., y Pintrich, P. R. (1994). Regulating motivation and cognition in the classroom: The role of self-schemas and self-regulatory strategies. In D. H. Schunk y B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-regulation of learning and performance: Issues and educational applications (pp. 101-124). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum

González Cabanach, R., Valle Arias, A., Núñez Pérez, J. C., y González García, J. A. (1996). Una aproximación teórica al concepto de metas académicas y su relación con la motivación escolar. Psicothema, 8 (1). Recuperado de: http://digibuo.uniovi.es/dspace/bitstream/10651/29565/1/Psicothema.1996.8.1.45-61.pdf

González-Pienda, J. A. (2003). El rendimiento escolar. Un análisis de las variables que lo condicionan. Revista Galego-Portuguesa de psicoloxía e Educación, 7 (8), 247-258. Recuperado de: https://ruc.udc.es/dspace/bitstream/handle/2183/6952/?sequence=1

Kane, M. J., y Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 132(1), 47. Recuperado de: http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.323.3307yrep=rep1ytype=pdf

Karlen, Y. (2016). Differences in students' metacognitive strategy knowledge, motivation, and strategy use: A typology of self-regulated learners. The Journal of Educational Research, 109(3), 253-265. doi: 10.1080/00220671.2014.942895

Konrad, S. Č. (2015). “How and Why should i study?”: Metacognitive Learning strategies and Motivational beliefs as important Predictors of Academic Performance of student teachers. The new Educational Review, 42(4), 239-250 doi: 10.15804/tner.2015.42.4.20

Landine, J. y Stewart, J. (1998). Relationship Between Metacognition, Motivation, Locus of Control, Self-Efficacy, and Academic Achievement. Canadian Journal of Counselling, 32(3), 200-212. Recuperado de: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ576966.pdf

Marulis, L. M., Baker, S. T., y Whitebread, D. (2018). Integrating metacognition and executive function to enhance young children’s perception of and agency in their learning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.017

McCombs, B. L. (1984). Processes and skills underlying continuing intrinsic motivation to learn: Toward a definition of motivational skills training interventions. Educational Psychologist, 19 (4), 199-218. doi: 10.1080/00461528409529297

McCombs, B. L. (1988). Motivational skills training: Combining metacognitive, cognitive, and affective learning strategies. In Learning and study strategies (pp. 141-169). doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-742460-6.50015-3

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., y Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49 –100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Mok, Y. F., Fan, R. M. T., y Pang, N. S. K. (2007). Developmental patterns of school students' motivational‐and cognitive‐metacognitive competencies. Educational Studies, 33(1), 81-98. doi: 10.1080/03055690600948281

Molfese, V. J., Molfese, P. J., Molfese, D. L., Rudasill, K. M., Armstrong, N., y Starkey, G. (2010). Executive function skills of 6–8 year olds: Brain and behavioral evidence and implications for school achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 116–125. doi:10.1016/ j.cedpsych.2010.03.004

Núñez Pérez, J. C., González García, J. A., García Rodríguez, M. S., González-Pumariega Solís, S., Roces Montero, C., Álvarez Pérez, L., y González Torres, M. D. C. (1998). Estretegias de aprendizaje, autoconcepto y rendimiento académico. Psicothema, 10 (1) 97-109. Recuperado de: http://digibuo.uniovi.es/dspace/handle/10651/29244

Paris, S.G., y Newman, R.S. (1990) Development Aspects of Self-Regulated Learning, Educational Psychologist, 25(1), 87-102. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2501_7

Park, S. B., y Bae, S. J. (2014). Different routes to metacognitive judgments: The role of accuracy motivation. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(3), 307-319. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2013.09.002

Pintrich, P. (2000). An achievement goal theory perspective on issues in motivation terminology, theory, and research. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25 (1), 92-104. doi: https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1017

Pintrich, P. y De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of educational psychology, 82 (1), 33-40. doi: https://doi.org/O022-O663/90/$00.75

Portellano, J.A., Martínez-Arias, R. y Zumárraga, L., (2011). ENFEN. Evaluación Neuropsicológica de las funciones ejecutivas en niños. Madrid, España: TEA Ediciones, S.A.U

Roebers, C. M. (2017). Executive function and metacognition: Towards a unifying framework of cognitive self-regulation. Developmental Review, 45, 31-51. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2017.04.001

Schunk, D. H. (1996). Goal and self-evaluative influences during children’s cognitive skill learning. American educational research journal, 33(2), 359-382. doi: https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312033002359

Schunk, D. H. (2005). Self-regulated learning: The educational legacy of Paul R. Pintrich. Educational psychologist, 40(2), 85-94. doi: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4002_3

Short, E. J., y Weissberg-Benchell, J. A. (1989). The triple alliance for learning: Cognition, metacognition, and motivation. In Cognitive strategy research, (pp. 33-63). Springer, New York, NY. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-8838-8_2

Sperling, R.; Howard, B.; Staley, R. y DuBois, N. (2004). Metacognition and self-regulated learning constructs. Educational Research and Evaluation, 10 (2), 117-139. doi: https://doi.org/1380-3611/04/1002-117

Spiess, M. A., Meier, B., y Roebers, C. M. (2016). Development and longitudinal relationships between children’s executive functions, prospective memory, and metacognition. Cognitive development, 38, 99-113. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.02.003

Suárez, J., y Fernández, A. (2011). A model of how motivational strategies related to the expectative component affect cognitive and metacognitive strategies. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 9(2), 641-658. Recuperado de: http://www.investigacion-psicopedagogica.org/revista/articulos/24/english/Art_24_550.pdf

Sungur, S. (2007). Contribution of motivational beliefs and metacognition to students' performance under consequential and nonconsequential test conditions. Educational Research and Evaluation, 13(2), 127-142. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/13803610701234898

Sungur, S. (2007a). Modeling the relationships among students' motivational beliefs, metacognitive strategy use, and effort regulation. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 51(3), 315-326. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/00313830701356166

Tapia, J. A., y Ferrer, J. S. (1992). El cuestionario MAPE-I: Motivación hacia el aprendizaje. En Motivar en la adolescencia: Teoría, evaluación e intervención (pp. 53-91). Madrid, España: Ediciones de la Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.

Published
01-01-2021
How to Cite
Lara Nieto-Márquez, N., Garcia-Sinausia, S., & Pérez Nieto, M. Ángel. (2021). Links between motivation and metacognition and achievement in cognitive performance among primary school pupils. Anales de Psicología / Annals of Psychology, 37(1), 51–60. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.383941
Issue
Section
Developmental and Educational Psychology

Most read articles by the same author(s)