Los precursores cognitivos tempranos de la lectura inicial: un modelo de aprendizaje en niños de 6 a 8 años
Abstract
Early literacy studies analyzes the antecedent relationships between cognitive skills in preschool education and achievement of reading during the instructional learning process. This study describes the predictive capacity of certain cognitive abilities in the early instructional stages of Spanish learning. The participants of this study was 362 Spanish children evaluated in 2nd and 3rd year of Early Childhood Education (children aged 4 to 6 years) at the first time.
The instruments used in the assessments of language skills have been the Beginning Reading Battery, the Reading Process Assessment Battery, The Rapid Automatized Naming Test and the Reading and Writing Test in Spanish. The results delimited a model of structural equations for the prediction of initial reading performance in 1st and 2nd year of Primary Education (6 to 8 years). The model reached, Learning Model in the Initial Reading, delimited the relations between the oral language skills and the skills related to the code that acts as precursors of the initial reader performance. The performance in the task of letters-naming speed at 2nd and 3rd of Early Childhood Education was presented as the best precursor of reading efficiency at 1st and 2nd year of Primary Education. The findings of study make a significant contribution to early literacy research in Spanish as a reference to develop of pedagogical actions for the achievement of reader success.
Downloads
References
Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Lee, J. (2010). Kindergarten predictors of second versus eighth grade reading comprehension impairments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(4), 332-345. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219410369067
Cuetos, F., Rodríguez, B., Ruano, E., & Arribas, D. (2007). PROLEC-R. Batería de evaluación de los procesos lectores. Revisada. Madrid: TEA.
Defior, S., Fonseca, L., Gottheil, B., Aldrey, A., Jiménez-Fernández, M. P., Rosa, G., & Serrano, F. D. (2006). Test de Lectura y Escritura en Español. Buenos Aires, Argentiva: Paidós.
Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Poe, M. D. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 465-481. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.3.465
Catts, H. W., Herrera, S., Nielsen, D. C., & Bridges, M. S. (2015). Early prediction of reading comprehension within the simple view framework. Reading and Writing, 28(9), 1407-1425. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9576-x
Evans, M. A., Bell, M., Shaw, D., Moretti, S., & Page, J. (2006). Letter names, letter sounds and phonological awareness: An examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children. Reading and Writing, 19(9), 959-989. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9026-x
Ferroni, M., Diuk, B., & Mena, M. (2016). Desarrollo de la lectura y la escritura de palabras con ortografía compleja: sus predictores. Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana, 34(2), 253-271. doi: https://doi.org/10.12804/apl34.2.2016.04
Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 566-580. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566
González-Valenzuela, M. J., Díaz-Giráldez, F., & López-Montiel, M. D. (2016). Cognitive predictors of word and pseudoword reading in Spanish first-grade children. Frontiers in psychology, 7(774),1-12. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00774
Kirby, J. R., Desrochers, A., Roth, L., & Lai, S. S. (2008). Longitudinal predictors of word reading development. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 49(2), 103-110. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0708-5591.49.2.103
Frost, J., Madsbjerg, S., Niedersoe, J., Olofsson, A., & Sorensen, P. M. (2005). Semantic and phonological skills in predicting reading development: From 3–16 years of age. Dyslexia, 11(2), 79-92. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.292
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 765-778. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015956
Lonigan, C. J. (2006). Development, assessment, and promotion of preliteracy skills. Early Education and Development, 17(1), 91-114. doi: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1701_5
Lonigan, C. J., Schatschneider, C., Westberg, L., & The National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Identification of children’s skills and abilities linked to later outcomes in reading, writing, and spelling (Developing early literacy: A scientific synthesis of early literacy development and implications for intervention). Jessup, MD: National Institute for Literacy & The Partnership
Manis, F., Doi, L., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and ortographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 325-333, 374. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940003300405
Mol, S., Bus, A., & De Jong, M. (2008). Added Value of Dialogic Parent-Child Book Readings: A Meta-Analysis. Early Education & Development, 19(1), 7-26. doi: https://doi.org/0.1080/10409280701838603
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M.J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rhymes, vocabulary and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665-681. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.665
Neuhaus, G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in rapid automatized naming (RAN) and their relation to reading. Journal of experimental child psychology, 78(4), 359-373. doi: https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000.2576
Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(2), 91-121. doi: https://doi.org.10.1080/10888438.2010.529219
Paris, S. G. (2005). Reinterpreting the development of reading skills. Reading research quarterly, 40(2), 184-202. doi: https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.40.2.3
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J., Francis, D., Carlson, C., & Foorman, B. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(2), 265-282.
Sellés, P., Martínez, T., & Vidal-Abarca. E. (2010). Batería de Inicio a la lectura: Diseño y características psicométricas. Bordón, 62(2), 137-160.
Silvén, M., Poskiparta, E., Niemi, P., & Voeten, M. (2007). Precursors of reading skill from infancy to first grade in finnish: Continuity and change in a highly inflected language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 516-531. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.516
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 934-947.
Suárez-Coalla, P., García-de-Castro, M., & Cuetos, F. (2013). Variables predictoras de la lectura y la escritura en castellano. Infancia y aprendizaje, 36(1), 77-89. doi: https://doi.org/doi.org/10.1174/021037013804826537
Torppa, M., Lyytinen, P., Erskine, J., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(4), 308–321.
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69(3), 848-72.
Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. (2003). Rapid Automatized Naming Tests. Greenville, SC: Super Duper.
The articles and scientific documents published in RIE abide the following conditions:
1. The Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Murcia (the publisher) has the property rights (copyright) of all the documents published and allows the reuse under the user’s license indicated in point 2.
2. All documents are published in the digital edition of RIE under a Creative Commons Reconocimiento-NoComercial-SinObraDerivada 3.0 España (legal document) license. These documents can be copied, used, distributed, communicated and explained publicly if: i) the author(s) and its original source of publishing (magazine, publisher and URL of the document) are cited; ii) it is not used for commercial purpose; iii) the existence and the specifications about this license are mentioned.
3. Auto-archive’s conditions. The authors are allowed and encouraged to digitally distribute the pre-print versions (a version before evaluation) and/or post-print (a version that it is already evaluated and accepted to its publication). This promotes circulation and distribution earlier and can increase the citations and significance within the academic community.