Perceptual-motor expertise and cognition in sport: from ecological and dynamic approach to enaction
Abstract
In this paper three alternative theoretical streams to cognitivism, with particular emphasis on enactivism are discussed in order to explain the sport skill acquisition process and human control of movement. In the first part, main concepts of ecological psychology are outlined, like movement regulation, direct perception or mutual interdependence between perception and action. Afterwards, using dynamical model as background, some concepts of motor skill coordination are explained (i.e., constraints or movement emergency). The next section will be focused on enactive approach as a conceptual extension from cognitive science that transcends the other paradigms. From this orientation, it calls for a mind-body fusion of the athlete, which is inseparable to the environment and opposing the prevailing dualism and reductionism. Finally, some guidelines and applications enactive research that are currently being developed are presented.
Downloads
References
Abernethy, B. (1998). Perception-action relationships and the development of motor expertise. Science et Motricité, 28, 6-8.
Abernethy, B. y Sparrow, W. A. (1992). The rise and fall of dominant paradigms in motor behaviour research. En J. J. Summers (Ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 3-45). North Holland: Elsevier Science.
Araújo D., Davids K. y Hristovski R. (2006). The ecological dynamics of decision making in sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7 (6), 653-676.
Avilés, C., Ruiz, L. M. y Benguigui, N. (2006) ¿Qué conocemos sobre el
comportamiento anticipatorio de los jugadores de tenis expertos durante el resto de un primer servicio? En D. Cabello, A. Lees, G. Torres y I. Roldán (Eds.), Colección Congresos nº 2: IV World Congress of Science and Racket Sports. Madrid: Alto Rendimiento.
Bardy, B. G. (2006). Perception et régulation du mouvement humain: Plaidoyer pour une biologie physique. Journal de la Société de Biologie, 200 (2), 135-143.
Bardy, B. G. (2011). Learning new skills in multimodal enactive environments. En B. G. Bardy, J. Lagarde y D. Mottet (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2011 International SKILLS Conference. Paris: EDP Sciences.
Bardy, B. G., Delignières, D., Lagarde, J., Mottet, D. y Zelic, G. (2010). An enactive approach to perception-action and skill acquisition in virtual reality environments. Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics. Miami, FL, USA.
Bardy, B. G. y Mottet, D. (2006). Introduction. Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Enactive Interfaces: Enaction & Complexity. Montpellier, France. Recuperado de http://www.m2h.euromov.eu/documents/congres/congre_7.pdf
Beek, P. J., Jacobs, D., Daffertshofer, A. y Huys, R. (2003). Expert performance in sport: Views on expert performance in sport from the joint perspectives of ecological psychology and dynamical systems theory. En J. L. Starkes y K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), Expert performance in sports: Advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 321-344). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Benguigui, N. (1997). Effet de la pratique d’un sport de balle sur le développement des processus perceptifs impliqués dans les actions d’interception. Thèse de doctorat non publiée. Faculté des sciences du sport, Université de Poitiers, Poitiers, France.
Berthoz, A. (1997). Le Sens du Mouvement. Paris: Odile Jacob.
Bootsma, R. J. (1993). L'approche écologique de la perception et l'action. Actes des Journées ACAPS "Neurosciences des A.P.S.", Poitiers, France.
Bootsma R. J. (1994). Le couplage Perception-Action dans les activités d’interception. Document inédit. Habilitation à diriger des Recherches. Marseille: Université de la Méditerranée.
Bootsma, R. J., Houbiers, M., Whiting, H. T. A. y Van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1991). Acquiring an attacking forehand drive: The effects of static and dynamic environmental conditions. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 62, 276-284.
Bootsma, R. J. y Van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1990). Timing an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 21-29.
Bruner, J. S. (1980). Investigaciones sobre el desarrollo cognitivo. Madrid: Pablo del Río.
Carlton, L. G., Carlton, M. J. & Kim, K. H. (1991). Visual processing time with changing environmental conditions. Unpublished raw data.
Carlton, L. G. (1992). Visual processing time and the control of movement. En L. Proteau y D. Elliot (Eds.), Vision and motor control (pp. 3-31). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Colley, A. (1989). Learning motor skills: Integrative cognition and action. En A. Colley y J. Beech (Eds.), Acquisition and Performance of Cognitive Skills (pp. 167-189). Chichester: John Wiley y Sons.
Crognier, L. y Féry, A. (2007). 40 ans de recherches sur l'anticipation en tennis: Une revue critique. Science et Motricité, 62, 9-36.
Davids, K., Button, C. y Bennett, S. (2008). Dynamics of skill acquisition: A constraints-led approach. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Davids, K., Handford, C. y Williams, M. (1994). The natural physical alternative to cognitive theories of motor behavior: An invitation for interdisciplinary research in sports science? Journal of sports Sciences, 12, 495-528.
Davids, K., Williams, A.M., Button, C. y Court, M. (2001). An integrative modeling approach to the study of intentional movement behaviour. En R. N. Singer, H. Hausenblas y C. Janelle, (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (pp. 144-173). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Dewey, J. (1929). Experience and nature. (2a ed.). New York: Dover
Di Paolo, E., Rohde, M. y De Jaegher, H. (2010). Horizons for the enactive mind: Values, social interaction, and play. En J. Stewart, O. Gapenne y E. Di Paolo (Eds.), Enaction: Towards a New Paradigm for Cognitive Science (pp. 33-87). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Farrow, D. y Abernethy, B. (2003). Do expertise and the degree of perception-action coupling affect natural anticipatory performance? Perception, 32, 1127-1139.
Faste, H., Todaro, E. y Bergamasco, M. (2008). Kinesthetic thinking: Heightening enactive experience. Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Enactive Interfaces, Pisa.
Gravel, P. (2006, Junio 5). Concept d’« enaction»: La connaissance du monde passé par l’action. Le Devoir, p. A4.
Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. (Trabajo original publicado en 1979).
Handford, C., Davids, K., Bennett, S. y Button, C. (1997). Skill acquisition in sport: Some applications of an evolving practice ecology. Journal of Sports Sciences, 15, 621-640.
Havelange V., Lenay C. y Stewart J. (2003). Les représentations: Mémoire externe et objets techniques. Intellectica, 35, 115-131.
Ibáñez-Gijón, J., Travieso, D. y Jacobs, D. M. (2011). El enfoque ecológico como marco conceptual y metodológico para el diseño de programas de entrenamiento deportivo. Revista de Psicología del Deporte, 20, 667-688.
Jacobs, D. M. y Michaels, C. F. (2007). Direct learning. Ecological Psychology, 19, 321-349
Keil, D., Holmes, P., Bennett, S., Davids, K. y Smith, N. (2000). Theorie and pratique in sport psychology and motor behavior needs to be constrained by integrative modelling of brain behavior. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 433-443.
Laurent, E. y Ripoll, H. (2009). Extending the rather unnoticed Gibsonian view that ‘perception is cognitive’: Development of the enactive approach to perceptual-cognitive expertise. En D. Araújo, H. Ripoll y M. Raab (Eds.), Perspectives on Cognition and Action in Sport (pp. 133-146). Hauppauge, New York: Nova Publishers.
Lee, D. y Reddish, P. E. (1981). Plummeting gannets: A paradigm of ecological optics. Nature, 293, 293–294.
Marr, D. (1985). La visión. Madrid: Alianza.
Masciotra, D., Roth, W.-M. y Morel, D. (2008). Énaction: Apprendre et enseigner en situation. Bruxelles: De Boeck.
Maturana, H. y Varela, F. (1996). El árbol del conocimiento. Las bases biológicas del conocimiento humano. Madrid: Debate.
Maturana, H. y Varela, F. (2004). De máquinas y seres vivos. Autopoiesis: La organización de lo vivo. Buenos Aires: Editorial Universitaria y Editorial Lumen.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1985). Fenomenología de la Percepción. Barcelona: Planeta-Agostini
Michaels, C. F. y Carello C. (1981). Direct Perception. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Michaels, C. F. y Beek, P. (1995). The state of ecological psychology. Ecological Psychology, 7, 259-278.
Milner, A. D. y Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford, England UK: Oxford University Press.
Moe, V. F. (2005) A Philosophical Critique of Classical Cognitivism in Sport: From information processing to bodily background knowledge. Journal of the Philosophy of Sport, 32, 155-183.
Montagne, G. (2005). Prospective control in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 36, 127-150.
Montagne, G., Bastin, J. y Jacobs, D. M. (2008). What is visual anticipation, and how much does it rely on the dorsal stream? International Journal of Sport Psychology, 39(2), 149-156.
Moreno, F. J. y Ordoño, E. M. (2009). Aprendizaje motor y síndrome general de adaptación. Motricidad. European Journal of Human Movement, 22, 1-21.
Newell, K. M. (1985). Coordination control and skill. En D. Goodman, R. B. Wilberg y I. M. Franks (Eds.), Differing perspectives on motor learning, memory and control (pp. 295-317). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. En M. G. Wade y H. T. A. Whiting (Eds.), Motor Development in children: Aspects of coordination and control (pp. 341-360). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.
Nistch, J. R. y Munzert, J. (2002). Aspectos del entrenamiento de la técnica desde la perspectiva de la teoría de la acción: Aproximaciones a un modelo integrador. En J. R. Nitsch, A. Neumaier, H. Mareales y J. Mester (Eds.), Entrenamiento de la técnica (pp. 129-191). Barcelona: Paidotribo.
Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Noë, A. (2010). Fuera de la cabeza: Por qué no somos el cerebro: Y otras lecciones de la biología de la consciencia. Barcelona: Kairós.
Norman, J. (2002). Two visual systems and two theories of perception: An attempt to reconcile the constructivist and ecological approaches. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25 (1), 73-144.
O'Regan, J. K. y Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939-1031.
Oullier, O., Lagarde, J. F., Jantzen, K. J. y Kelso, J. A. S. (2006). Coordination dynamics: (in)stability and metastability in the behavioural and neural systems. Journal de la Société de Biologie, 200(2), 145-167.
Paillard, J. (1991). The cognitive penetrability of sensorimotor mechanisms: A key problem in sport research. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 22, 244-250.
Palut, Y., Zanone, P. G. y Gurdjos, P. (2004). Modes de déplacement spontané en tennis. Science & Motricité, 51, 69-83.
Pasquinelli, E. (2007). Enactive Knowledge. En A. Luciani y C. Cadoz (Eds). Enaction and enactive interfaces, a handbook of terms (pp. 93-95). Grenoble: Enactive Systems Books.
Penelaud, O. (2010). Le paradigme de l’énaction aujourd’hui: Apports et limites d’une théorie cognitive « révolutionnaire ». Revue Transdisciplinaire de Plasticité Humaine, 18, 1-38. Recuperado de http://plasticites-sciences-arts.org/PLASTIR/Penelaud%20P18.pdf
Poincairé, H. (1907). La science et l’hyphotesis. Paris: Flammarion.
Ripoll, H. (2009). What is the impact of knowledge on player’s behaviour? En D. Araújo, H. Ripoll y M. Raab (Eds.), Perspectives on Cognition and Action in Sport (pp. 89-93). New York: Nova Science.
Ruiz, L. M. (2003a). Competencia motriz, dinamismo y complejidad en educación física: Parte I. Retos: Nuevas tendencias en Educación Física, Deporte y Recreación, 5, 39-43.
Ruiz, L. M. (2003b). Competencia motriz, dinamismo y complejidad en educación física: Parte II. Retos: Nuevas tendencias en Educación Física, Deporte y Recreación, 6, 5-12.
Ruiz, L. M., Sánchez, M., Durán, J. y Jiménez, C. (2006). Los expertos en el deporte: Su estudio y análisis desde una perspectiva psicológica. Anales de psicología, 22(1), 132-142.
Savelsbergh, G. J. P., van der Kamp, J., Oudejans, R. R. D. y Scott, M. A. (2004). Perceptual learning is mastering perceptual degrees of freedom. En A. M. Williams y N. J. Hodges (Eds.), Skill Acquisition in Sport: Research Theory and Practice (pp. 374–389). London: Taylor& Francis.
Schmidt, R. A. (1991). Motor learning and performance: From principles to practice. Champaign, IL: Human kinetics.
Stewart, J., Gapenne, O. y Di Paolo, E. (2010). Enaction: Towards a new paradigm for cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Stoffregen, T. A., Bardy, B. G. y Mantel, B. (2006). Affordances in the design of enactive systems. Virtual Reality, 10, 4-10.
Summers, J. J. (2004). A historical perspective on skill acquisition. En A. M. Williams y N. J. Hodges (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory and practice (pp. 1–26). London: Routledge.
Temprado, J. J. (1994). Méthodes de chronométrie mentale et traitement de l'information. Science et Motricité, 22, 23-33.
Temprado, J. J. y Laurent, M. (1995). Approches cognitive et écologique de l'apprentissage des habilités motrices en sport. En H. Ripoll, J. Bilard, M. Durand, J. Keller, M. Levêque y P. Therme. (Eds.), Psychologie du sport: Questions actuelles. (pp. 223-236). Paris: Revue EPS.
Temprado, J. J. y Montagne, G. (2001). Les coordinations perceptivo-motrices. Paris: Armand Colin.
Todaro, E., Faste, H., Portillo-Rodriguez, O. y Bergamasco, M. (2007). The hand dance: A didactic performance platform. Paper presented at the International Cultural Heritage Informatics Meeting, Toronto.
Varela F. (1996). Invitation aux sciences cognitives. Paris: Éditions du Seuil.
Varela, F. (2000). El fenómeno de la vida. Santiago: Dolmen.
Varela, F., Thompson, E. y Rosch, E. (2005). De cuerpo presente: Las ciencias cognitivas y la experiencia humana. Barcelona: Gedisa.
Vickers, J. (2009). Advances in coupling perception and action: The quiet eye as a bidirectional link between gaze, attention, and action. Progress in Brain Research, 174, 279-288.
Warren, W. H. (2006a). The dynamics of perception and action. Psychological Review, 113 (2), 358-389.
Warren, W. H. (2006b). Enactive Knowledge: Sensory-motor expectancies or perception-action invariants? Paper presented at the 3rd International Conference on Enactive Interfaces: Enaction & Complexity, Montpellier.
Whiting, H. T. A. (1984). Human motor actions: Bernstein reassessed. Amsterdam: North Holland.
Williams, A. M. y Davids, K. (1998). Perceptual expertise in sport: Research, theory and practice. En H. Steinberg, I. Cockerill, y A. Dewey (Eds.), What sport psychologist do (pp. 48-57). Leicester, UK: British Psychological Society.
Williams, A. M., Davids, K. y Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual perception and action in sport. London: E. & F. N. Spon.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625-636.
Withagen, R. y Chemero, A. (2009). Naturalizing perception: Developing the Gibsonian approach to perception along evolutionary lines. Theory & Psychology, 19, 363-389.
The works published in this journal are subject to the following terms:
1. The Publications Service of the University of Murcia (the publisher) retains the property rights (copyright) of published works, and encourages and enables the reuse of the same under the license specified in paragraph 2.
© Servicio de Publicaciones, Universidad de Murcia, 2022
2. The works are published in the online edition of the journal under a Creative Commons Reconocimiento-CompartirIgual 4.0 (legal text). You can copy, use, distribute, transmit and publicly display, provided that: i) you cite the author and the original source of publication (journal, editorial and URL of the work), ii) are not used for commercial purposes, iii ) mentions the existence and specifications of this license.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.
3. Conditions of self-archiving. Is allowed and encouraged the authors to disseminate electronically pre-print versions (version before being evaluated and sent to the journal) and / or post-print (version reviewed and accepted for publication) of their works before publication, as it encourages its earliest circulation and diffusion and thus a possible increase in its citation and scope between the academic community. RoMEO Color: Green.